-
sensation
the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. (p. 180)
-
perception
he process of organizing and interpreting sensory information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events. (p. 180)
-
bottom-up processing:
analysis that begins with the sensory receptors and works up to the brain’s integration of sensory information. (p. 180)
i.e.. bottom–up processing enables our sensory systems to detect the lines, angles, and colors that form the horses, rider, and surroundings.
-
top-down processing:
information processing guided by higher-level mental processes, as when we construct perceptions drawing on our experience and expectations. (p. 180
i.e.. Using top–down processing, we consider the painting’s title, notice the apprehensive expressions, and then direct our attention to aspects of the painting that will give those observations meaning.
-
psychophysics:
the study of relationships between the physical characteristics of stimuli, such as their intensity, and our psychological experience of them. (p. 181)
-
absolute threshold:
the minimum stimulation needed to detect a particular stimulus 50 percent of the time. (p. 181)
-
subliminal:
below one’s absolute threshold for conscious awareness. (p. 181)
-
yes
Can we sense stimuli below our absolute thresholds? In one sense, the answer is clearly yes
-
priming:
the activation, often unconsciously, of certain associations, thus predisposing one’s perception, memory, or response. (pp. 182, 274
-
Much of our information processing occurs automatically, out of sight, off the radar screen of our conscious mind.
-
Their conclusion:
“Subliminal procedures offer little or nothing of value to the marketing practitioner
-
difference threshold:
the minimum difference between two stimuli required for detection 50 percent of the time. We experience the difference threshold as a just noticeable difference (or jnd). (p. 182)
-
Weber’s law:
the principle that, to be perceived as different, two stimuli must differ by a constant minimum percentage (rather than a constant amount). (p. 183)
-
sensory adaptation:
diminished sensitivity as a consequence of constant stimulation. (p. 183)
-
eye movement
unnoticed by us, our eyes are always moving, flitting from one spot to another enough to guarantee that stimulation on the eyes’ receptors continually changes
-
Our sensory receptors
are alert to novelty; bore them with repetition and they free our attention for more important things. Stinky or heavily perfumed people don’t notice their odor because, like you and me, they adapt to what’s constant and only detect change.
-
We perceive the world not exactly as it is, but as it is useful for us to perceive it.
-
Sensory thresholds and adaptation
are only two of the commonalities shared by the senses. All our senses receive sensory stimulation, transform it into neural information, and deliver that information to the brain
-
light/vision
Our eyes, for example, receive light energy and transform it into neural messages that our brain then processes into what we consciously see.
-
scientifically speaking
what strikes our eyes is not color but pulses of energy that our visual system perceives as color. What we see as visible light (Figure 6.5) is but a thin slice of the whole spectrum of electromagnetic energy ranging from the imperceptibly short waves of gamma rays to the long waves of radio transmission. Other organisms are sensitive to differing portions of this spectrum. Bees, for instance, cannot see red but can see ultraviolet light.
-
wavelength:
the distance from the peak of one light or sound waveto the peak of the next. Electromagnetic wavelengths vary from the short blips of cosmic rays to the long pulses of radio transmission. (p.185)
-
hue:
the dimension of color that is determined by the wavelength of light; what we know as the color names blue, green, and so forth. (p. 185)
-
intensity:
the amount of energy in a light or sound wave, which we perceive as brightness or loudness, as determined by the wave’s amplitude. (p. 185)
-
light and eyes
Light enters the eye through the cornea, which protects the eye and bends light to provide focus . The light then passes through the pupil, a small adjustable opening surrounded by the iris, a colored muscle that adjusts light intake. The iris dilates or constricts in response to light intensity and even to inner emotions.
-
retina:
the light-sensitive inner surface of the eye, containing the receptor rods and cones plus layers of neurons that begin the processing of visual information. (p. 186)
-
accommodation:
the process by which the eye’s lens changes shape to focus near or far objects on the retina. (p. 186)
-
millions of receptor cells convert particles of light energy into neural impulses and forward those to the brain
-
rods:
retinal receptors that detect black, white, and gray; necessary for peripheral and twilight vision, when cones don’t respond. (p. 186)
-
cones
retinal receptor cells that are concentrated near the center of the retina and that function in daylight or in well-lit conditions. The cones detect fine detail and give rise to color sensations. (p. 186)
-
optic nerve:
the nerve that carries neural impulses from the eye to the brain. (p. 187)
-
blind spot:
the point at which the optic nerve leaves the eye, creating a “blind” spot because no receptor cells are located there. (p.187)
-
fovea:
the central focal point in the retina, around which the eye’s cones cluster. (p. 188)
-
cones
Many cones have their own hotline to the brain—bipolar cells that help relay the cone’s individual message to the visual cortex, which devotes a large area to input from the fovea. These directconnections preserve the cones’ preciseinformation, making them better able to detect fine detail
-
cones to detail and color, and rods to faint light.
-
visual process
- At the entry level, the retina processes information before routing it via the thalamus to the brain’s cortex
- Any given retinal area relays its information to a corresponding location in the visual cortex, in the occipital lobe at the back of your brain
-
feature detectors: David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel (1979)
nerve cells in the brain that respond to specific features of a stimulus, such as shape, angle, or movement. (p. 188
-
One temporal lobe area just behind your right ear, for example, enables you to perceive faces. If this region were damaged, you might recognize other forms and objects
-
parallel processing:
- the processing of many aspects of a problem simultaneously; the brain’s natural mode of information processing for many functions, including vision. Contrasts with the step-by-step (serial) processing of most computers and of conscious problem solving. (p. 190)
- Studies of patients with brain damage suggest that the brain delegates the work of processing color, movement, form, and depth to different areas
-
brain damage
Others with stroke or surgery damage to their brain’s visual cortex have experienced blindsight, a localized area of blindness in part of their field of vision
-
color vision
Color, like all aspects of vision, resides not in the object but in the theater of our brains, as evidenced by our dreaming in color.
-
Young-Helmholtz trichromatic (three-color) theory:
the retina contains three different color receptors—one most sensitive to red, one to green, one to blue—which, when stimulated in combination, can produce the perception of any color. (p. 191)
-
3 color theory
- red, green, and blue
- those color blind lack blue or red or even both
- monochromatic (one-color) or dichromatic (two-color)
-
opponent-process theory:
the theory that opposing retinal processes (red-green, yellow-blue, white-black) enable color vision. For example, some cells are stimulated by green and inhibited by red; others are stimulated by red and inhibited by green. (p. 192)
-
Color processing occurs in two stages
The cones’ signals are then processed by the nervous system’s opponent-process cells, en route to the visual cortex.
-
wave length
The physical characteristic of light that determines the color we experience, such as blue or green, is
-
The blind spot in your retina is located in an area where
the optic nerve leaves the eye.
-
bright; color
Cones are the eye’s receptor cells that are especially sensitive to _______ light and are responsible for our _______ vision.
-
feature detectors
The brain cells that respond maximally to certain bars, edges, and angles
-
parallel processing.
The brain’s ability to process many aspects of an object or problem simultaneously
-
three types of color receptors; opponent-process cells
Two theories together account for color vision. The Young-Helmholtz theory shows that the eye contains _______, and the Hering theory accounts for the nervous system’s having _______.
-
For humans, vision is the major sense. More of our brain cortex is devoted to vision than to any other sense
-
audition:
- the sense or act of hearing. (p. 193)
- We are also remarkably attuned to variations in sounds. We easily detect differences among thousands of human voices:
-
frequency:
- the number of complete wavelengths that pass a point in a given time (for example, per second). (p. 193)
- pitch: a tone’s experienced highness or lowness; depends on frequency. (p. 193)
-
waves/frequency
Long waves have low frequency—and low pitch. Short waves have high frequency—and high pitch
-
sound is measured by decibels
Every 10 decibels correspond to a tenfold increase in sound intensity.
-
ears
The middle ear then transmits the eardrum’s vibrations through a piston made of three tiny bones (the hammer, anvil, and stirrup) to the cochlea, a snail-shaped tube in the inner ear. The incoming vibrations cause the cochlea’s membrane (the oval window) to vibrate, jostling the fluid that fills the tube (Figure 6.16b). This motion causes ripples in the basilar membrane, bending the hair cells lining its surface
-
hair cells.
hese “quivering bundles that let us hear” marvels at their “extreme sensitivity and extreme speed.”
-
touch
Other skin sensations are variations of the basic four (pressure, warmth, cold, and pain):
-
rubber-hand illusion
- Touch is not only a bottom-up property of your senses but also a top-down product of your brain and your expectations.
- still feel even though not touched
-
kinesthesis: [kin-ehs-THEE-sehs]
- the system for sensing the position and movement of individual body parts. (p. 197)
- mportant sensors in your joints, tendons, bones, and ears, as well as your skin sensors enable your kinesthesis
-
vestibular sense:
vestibular sense: the sense of body movement and position, including the sense of balance. (p. 197)
monitors your head’s (and thus your body’s) position and movement.
-
pain
There is no one type of stimulus that triggers pain (as light triggers vision). Instead, there are different nociceptors—sensory receptors that detect hurtful temperatures, pressure, or chemicals
-
gate-control theory
the theory that the spinal cord contains a neurological “gate” that blocks pain signals or allows them to pass on to the brain. The “gate” is opened by the activity of pain signals traveling up small nerve fibers and is closed by activity in larger fibers or by information coming from the brain. (p. 199)
-
psychologist Ronald Melzack and biologist Patrick Wall’s (1965, 1983) classic gate–control theory
the theory that the spinal cord contains a neurological “gate” that blocks pain signals or allows them to pass on to the brain. The “gate” is opened by the activity of pain signals traveling up small nerve fibers and is closed by activity in larger fibers or by information coming from the brain. (p. 199)
-
false pain
The brain can also create pain, as it does in people’s experiences of phantom limb sensations, when it misinterprets the spontaneous central nervous system activity that occurs in the absence of normal sensory input
-
biological influences
- activity in spinal cord's lare and small fibers
- genetic differences in endorphin production
- the brain's interpretation of CNS
-
social-cultural influences
- presence of others
- empathy for other's pain
- cultural expectations
-
psychosocial influences
- attention to pain
- learning based on experiences
- expectations
-
Controlling Pain
drugs, surgery, acupuncture, electrical stimulation, massage, exercise, hypnosis, relaxation training, and thought distraction.Even an inert placebo can help
-
TASTE
- sweet = energy source
- salt = pysiological processes
- sour= potential toxic acid
- bitter= potentially poisoneous
- umami = protiens for muscle
-
taste cont...
Taste receptors reproduce themselves every week or two, so if you burn your tongue with hot food it hardly matters. However, as you grow older, the number of taste buds decreases, as does taste sensitivity
-
taste without smell not the same
To savor a taste, we normally breathe the aroma through our nose—which is why eating is not much fun when you have a bad cold. Smell can also change our perception of taste:
-
sensory interaction:
the principle that one sense may influence another, as when the smell of food influences its taste. (p. 202)
-
McGurk effect
Seeing the mouth movements for ga while hearing ba, we may perceive da—a
-
sense perception
Sensory interaction can also affect vision and touch. A weak flicker of light that we have trouble perceiving becomes more visible when accompanied by a short burst of sound
-
SMELL
odor molecules bind to different receptor arrays, producing the 10,000 odors we can detect
-
scents
people come to like that scent, which helps explain why people in the United States tend to like the smell of wintergreen (which they associate with candy and gum) more than do those in Great Britain (where it often is associated with medicine)
-
smell-cortex/limbic system
A hotline runs between the brain area receiving information from the nose and the brain’s ancient limbic centers associated with memory and emotion
-
cochlea
The snail-shaped tube in the inner ear, where sound waves are converted into neural activity, is called the
-
pressure
Of all the skin senses, only ________ has its own identifiable receptor cells.
-
inner ear.
The vestibular sense monitors the body’s position and movement. Vestibular sense receptors are located in the
-
The gate-control theory of pain proposes that
small spinal cord nerve fibers conduct most pain signals.
-
sensory interaction.
A food’s smell or aroma can greatly enhance its taste. This is an example of
-
gestalt:
an organized whole. Gestalt psychologists emphasized ourtendency to integrate pieces of information into meaningful wholes. (p.205)
-
Necker cube.
we view them all together, we see a whole, a form that psychologists call a
-
figure-ground:
the organization of the visual field into objects (the figures) that stand out from their surroundings (the ground). (p. 205)
-
grouping:
- the perceptual tendency to organize stimuli into coherent groups. (p. 206)
- Proximity We group nearby figures together, as in Figure 6.28. We see three sets of two lines, not six separate lines.
-
Similarity
We group similar figures together. We see the triangles and circles as vertical columns of similar shapes, not as horizontal rows of dissimilar shapes.
-
Continuity
We perceive smooth, continuous patterns rather than discontinuous ones. The pattern in the lower-left corner of Figure 6.28 could be a series of alternating semicircles, but we perceive it as two continuous lines—one wavy, one straight.
-
Connectedness
Because they are uniform and linked, we perceive each set of two dots and the line between them as a single unit.
-
Closure
We fill in gaps to create a complete, whole object. Thus we assume that the circles (below left) are complete but partially blocked by the (illusory) triangle. Add nothing more than little line segments that close off the circles (below right) and now your brain stops constructing a triangle.
-
depth perception:
the ability to see objects in three dimensions although the images that strike the retina are two-dimensional; allows us to judge distance. (p. 207)
-
visual cliff:
a laboratory device for testing depth perception in infants and young animals. (p. 207)
-
How do we transform two differing two-dimensional retinal images into a single three-dimensional perception?
The process begins with depth cues, some that depend on the use of two eyes, and others that are available to each eye separately.
-
binocular cues:
depth cues, such as retinal disparity, that depend on the use of two eyes. (p. 207)
-
retinal disparity:
a binocular cue for perceiving depth: By comparing images from the retinas in the two eyes, the brain computes distance—the greater the disparity (difference) between the two images, the closer the object. (p. 207)
-
3D
The creators of three-dimensional (3-D) movies simulate or exaggerate retinal disparity by photographing a scene with two cameras placed a few inches apart
-
monocular cues:
depth cues, such as interposition and linear perspective, available to either eye alone. (p. 208)
-
perceptual constancy:
perceiving objectsas unchanging (having consistent shapes, size, lightness, and color) even as illumination and retinal images change. (p. 210) »
-
Ames illusion
As the diagram reveals, the girls are actually about the same size, but the room is distorted
-
Perceived lightness
depends on relative luminance—the amount of light an object reflects relative to its surrounding
-
color constancy:
perceiving familiar objects as having consistent color, even if changing illumination alters the wavelengths reflected by the object. (p. 212)
-
perception cont..
Our perception of the color of a wall or of a streak of paint on a canvas is determined not just by the paint in the can but by the surrounding colors.
-
figure-ground
In listening to a concert, you follow the lead singer and perceive the other musicians as accompaniment; this illustrates the organizing principle of
-
grouping.
Our tendencies to fill in the gaps and to perceive a pattern as continuous are two different examples of the organizing principle called
-
Depth perception underlies our ability to
judge distances
-
linear perspective
Examples of monocular cues, which are available to either eye alone, include interposition and
-
perceptual constancy.
Perceiving tomatoes as consistently red, despite shifting illumination, is an example of
-
Remove cataracts that developed after early childhood, and a human, too, will enjoy normal vision.
In both humans and animals, a similar period of sensory restriction does no permanent harm if it occurs later in life.
-
perceptual adaptation:
in vision, the ability to adjust to an artificially displaced or even inverted visual field. (p. 214)
-
perceptual set:
- a mental predisposition to perceive one thing and not another. (p. 215)
- People perceive an adult-child pair as looking more alike when told they are parent and child
- Perceptual set can similarly influence what we hear
-
perception cont...
Even hearing sad rather than happy music can predispose people to perceive a sad meaning in spoken homophonic words—mourning rather than morning, die rather than dye, pain rather than pane
-
perception cont..
- The effects of perceptual set and context show how experience helps us construct perception
- is both learned and innate
-
extrasensory perception (ESP):
the controversial claim that perception can occur apart from sensory input, includes telepathy, clairvoyance, and precognition. (p. 218)
-
parapsychology:
the study of paranormal phenomena, including ESP and psychokinesis. (p. 218)
-
relavent claims of ESP
- Telepathy, or mind-to-mind communication: one person sending thoughts to another or perceiving another’s thoughts.
- Clairvoyance, or perceiving remote events, such as sensing that a friend’s house is on fire.
- Precognition, or perceiving future events, such as a political leader’s death or a sporting event’s outcome.
-
recognizing objects by sight
After surgery to restore vision, patients who had been blind from birth had difficulty
|
|